Linking birdsong and baby talk

    Updated: 2013-07-14 08:06

    By Tim Requarth and Meehan Crist(The New York Times)

      Print Mail Large Medium  Small

    Babies learn to speak months after they begin to understand language. As they are learning to talk, they babble, repeating the same syllable ("da-da-da") or combining syllables into a string ("da-do-da-do").

    But when babies babble, what are they doing? And why does it take them so long to speak?

    A surprising source is now providing some answers: songbirds.

    Researchers who focus on infant language and those who specialize in birdsong have published a new study suggesting that learning the transitions between syllables is the crucial bottleneck between babbling and speaking.

    "We've discovered a previously unidentified component of vocal development," said the lead author, Dina Lipkind, a psychology researcher at Hunter College in Manhattan. "What we're showing is that babbling is not only to learn sounds, but also to learn transitions between sounds."

    The results provide insight into language acquisition and may eventually help illuminate human speech disorders. At first, however, the scientists behind these findings were studying birds.

    "When I got into this, I never believed we were going to learn about human speech," said Ofer Tchernichovski, a birdsong researcher at Hunter College.

    Linking birdsong and baby talk

    He and Dr. Lipkind were teaching young zebra finches in soundproof boxes to switch the order of syllables in their songs. The birds could learn a new song only after practicing thousands of times a day for weeks. The roadblock seemed to be the transitions, as only the syllable order was changed.

    Their collaborator Kazuo Okanoya, then at the Riken Brain Science Institute in Japan, saw the same effect in Bengalese finches, which were living in a more natural environment, in large aviaries full of other finches.

    Dr. Tchernichovski and Gary Marcus, who studies infant language learning at New York University and who helped design the study, discussed the results in relation to human infants.

    They found that as babies learn a new syllable, they first tend to repeat it. Then, like the birds, they begin appending it to the beginning or end of syllable strings, eventually inserting it between other syllables.

    As with the birds, learning the transitions between new and old syllables is a painstaking process for babies. That could help explain why children continue to babble even as they begin to understand language.

    This study may also rekindle a debate over whether birdsong can be used to learn about human speech. The two may seem to have little in common, but recently, researchers have found many parallels.

    Birds and humans share molecular building blocks - including the FOXP2 gene, which made a big splash a decade ago when it was identified as the gene responsible for one human family's mysterious speech disorder.

    We seem to share brain structures crucial for song and speech, and both make phrases out of "syllables." Both also "babble" during a critical learning period; and both are "vocal learners," learning to sing or speak from a tutor or parent.

    With these parallels in mind, researchers are turning to birdsong as a model for human speech which is difficult to study. Vocal learning is rare among animals; not even humans' closest evolutionary relatives do so.

    Songbirds do. And researchers can do experiments with birds that they can't do with humans. As Dr. Lipkind put it, "You can't put babies in soundproof boxes."

    Sarah Woolley, a birdsong researcher at Columbia University in New York, agreed that the links were intriguing. "No one is saying birdsong is language," she said. "But there are so many parallels. We've got this opportunity to model vocal learning by testing how the rules work for birds and making predictions about how humans learn."

    But others question whether birdsong can ever really tell us much about human language. Still, songbird researchers are hopeful.

    "Roundworms are pretty evolutionarily distinct from humans, too," said a birdsong expert, Daniel Margoliash, of the University of Chicago. "I don't know about you, but when I look at a roundworm I don't think: 'Ah! Just like little Jimmy!' And yet they're a useful system."

    The New York Times

    (China Daily 07/14/2013 page11)

    精品人体无码一区二区三区 | 无码人妻精品一区二区三区久久 | 乱人伦中文视频在线| 在线高清无码A.| 最近中文字幕完整免费视频ww| 国产亚洲精品无码拍拍拍色欲| 无码国产精品一区二区免费式影视| 无码国模国产在线无码精品国产自在久国产 | 日韩人妻无码精品无码中文字幕| 国产精品无码久久四虎| 亚洲精品无码永久在线观看你懂的| 熟妇人妻中文字幕无码老熟妇| 国产精品无码DVD在线观看| 无码国产69精品久久久久网站| 亚洲欧洲中文日韩久久AV乱码| 国偷自产短视频中文版| 国产又爽又黄无码无遮挡在线观看| 无码视频在线观看| 亚洲成AV人片在线观看无码 | 最近更新免费中文字幕大全| 日本中文字幕在线不卡高清| 无码日韩人妻AV一区二区三区| 国产亚洲精品无码成人| 日韩一区二区三区无码影院 | 欧洲成人午夜精品无码区久久 | 无码囯产精品一区二区免费| 亚洲欧洲日产国码无码网站| 天堂Aⅴ无码一区二区三区| 免费a级毛片无码a∨免费软件 | 亚洲爆乳精品无码一区二区三区| 天堂Aⅴ无码一区二区三区| 国产在线无码精品电影网| 中文成人无码精品久久久不卡| 最近的2019免费中文字幕| 精品国产一区二区三区无码| 婷婷四虎东京热无码群交双飞视频 | 无码AV一区二区三区无码| 亚洲AV永久无码精品一区二区| 日韩精品无码Av一区二区| 日韩免费a级毛片无码a∨| 一本色道无码道在线|